References
[1]. Palmer, S. E. (1999). Color, consciousness, and the isomorphism constraint. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 22(6), 923-943.
[2]. Dougherty, R. F., Koch, V. M., Brewer, A. A., Fischer, B., Modersitzki, J., & Wandell, B. A. (2003). Visual field representations and locations of visual areas V1/2/3 in human visual cortex. Journal of vision, 3(10), 1-1.
[3]. Berlin, B., & Kay, P. (1991). Basic color terms: Their universality and evolution. Univ of California Press.
[4]. Kay, P., & Regier, T. (2003). Resolving the question of color naming universals. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 100(15), 9085-9089.
[5]. Roberson, D., Davies, I., & Davidoff, J. (2000). Color categories are not universal: replications and new evidence from a stone-age culture. Journal of experimental psychology: General, 129(3), 369.
[6]. Heider, E. R., & Olivier, D. C. (1972). The structure of the color space in naming and memory for two languages. Cognitive psychology, 3(2), 337-354.
[7]. Martin, A., & Chao, L. L. (2001). Semantic memory and the brain: structure and processes. Current opinion in neurobiology, 11(2), 194-201.
[8]. Mo, L., Xu, G., Kay, P., & Tan, L. H. (2011). Electrophysiological evidence for the left-lateralized effect of language on preattentive categorical perception of color. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 108(34), 14026-14030.
[9]. Eckhardt, G. M., & Houston, M. J. (2002). Cultural paradoxes reflected in brand meaning: McDonald's in Shanghai, China. Journal of International Marketing, 10(2), 68-82.